Hayali anı sendromu (psikolojik kurmacalar): Revizyonlar arasındaki fark

Vikipedi, özgür ansiklopedi
[kontrol edilmiş revizyon][kontrol edilmiş revizyon]
İçerik silindi İçerik eklendi
BendelacBOT (mesaj | katkılar)
Robbot (mesaj | katkılar)
84. satır: 84. satır:
[[ja:過誤記憶]]
[[ja:過誤記憶]]
[[pl:Fałszywe wspomnienie]]
[[pl:Fałszywe wspomnienie]]
[[ru:Псевдореминисценция]]
[[zh:假记忆体综合征]]
[[zh:假记忆体综合征]]

Sayfanın 17.45, 2 Eylül 2012 tarihindeki hâli

Sahte anı sendromu, psikolojik kurmacalar aslında hiç bir zaman var olmamış olmasına rağmen gerçek mekan ve gerçek kişilerle zihinde yaratılan bir hikayeleme senaryosu sonrası oluşan bir fantezi düş türüdür. genelde bir kaza veya trajik bir tranva olayı sonrası geçici şoka girme sürecinden çıktıktan sonraki süreçte olayın nasıl olduğu hakkında beyinin fikir üretme ve konuyu kişinin kendi savunma bilinç mekanizması noktasında olayı tam hatırlamadığı için olayda en çok kusurlu olanın kendi olabileceği düşüncesiyle diğerlerinin daha hatalı olduğu düşüncesiyle yaratılan ve genelde olay yerindeki diğer kişileri suçluyan ifadelere dönüşen durumlara sebep olabilmektedir.

ama daha çok genelde iyi anı anlamıyla uzak yerlerde (yurtdışı) oturan yakın akrabalarına düşkün çocukların özellikle uzak yerlerde çekilen fotoğraflardaki çocukları kendilerinin olduğunu sanmasıyla, sanki oralara da kısa bir süre yaşadığı yönde fikirlerin oluşması ve resimlerdeki gerçek hikayelerle örtüşür senaryolar ürettikleri olmuştur. genelde bu tarz durumlar hoş bir anı olarak aile içerisinde komik anı olarak bile bahsi geçebilmektedir.

bazılarına göre ise bu kuantum, paralel evrendeki diğer yaşamına 3. gözle bir bakış olarak metafizik yorumları bile söz konusu olabilmektedir.

False memory syndrome (FMS) describes a condition in which a person's identity and relationships are affected by memories which are factually incorrect but are strongly believed.[1] Peter J. Freyd originated the term,[2] which the False Memory Syndrome Foundation (FMSF) subsequently popularized.

False memories may be the result of recovered memory therapy, a term also defined by the FMSF in the early 1990s,[3] which describes a range of therapy methods that are prone to creating confabulations. Some of the influential figures in the genesis of the theory are forensic psychologist Ralph Underwager, psychologist Elizabeth Loftus and sociologist Richard Ofshe. False memory syndrome is not recognized as an official mental health diagnosis[4][5][6][7] but the principle that memories can be altered by outside influences is overwhelmingly accepted by scientists.[8][9][10][11]

Definition

False memory syndrome is defined as:

[A] condition in which a person's identity and interpersonal relationships are centered around a memory of traumatic experience which is objectively false but in which the person strongly believes. Note that the syndrome is not characterized by false memories as such. We all have memories that are inaccurate. Rather, the syndrome may be diagnosed when the memory is so deeply ingrained that it orients the individual's entire personality and lifestyle, in turn disrupting all sorts of other adaptive behavior...False Memory Syndrome is especially destructive because the person assiduously avoids confrontation with any evidence that might challenge the memory. Thus it takes on a life of its own, encapsulated and resistant to correction. The person may become so focused on memory that he or she may be effectively distracted from coping with the real problems in his or her life.[12] Emphasis in original

FMS is controversial.[13][14] It is not included in the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders. Paul R. McHugh, member of the FMSF, stated that the term was not adopted into the fourth version of the manual due to the pertinent committee being headed by believers in recovered memory.[2]

"Planted memories" differ from false memories in that the sufferer was very suggestible following or during hypnotherapy. Medical ethics forbid the practice. The sufferer may not be able to tell which memories are genuine and which are not. Confabulation resultant from the planting of these memories resolves temporary conflicts in the sufferer's mind but chronic damage may become permanently disabling, resulting in one or more personality disorders.[kaynak belirtilmeli]

"Indoctrinated memories" differ yet again in that the sufferer has been subtly reduced to believe propaganda. Mind control experiments in the Soviet Union, North Korea and China proved how far false memory syndrome can be used in the control of combat troops, who may come under orders to violate the rules of war specified under the Geneva Convention. Persons who have been indoctrinated severely enough may never recover any moral perspective, resulting in serious disciplinary problems.[kaynak belirtilmeli]

Recovered memory therapy

Recovered memory therapy is used to describe the therapeutic processes and methods that are believed to create false memories and false memory syndrome. These methods include hypnosis, sedatives and probing questions where the therapist believes repressed memories of childhood sexual abuse or other tramatic events are the cause of their client's problems.[15] The term is not listed in DSM-IV or used by any mainstream formal psychotherapy modality.[3]

Memory consolidation becomes a critical element of false memory and recovered memory syndromes. Once stored in the hippocampus, the memory may last for years or even for life, regardless that the memorized event never actually took place. Obsession to a particular false memory, planted memory, or indoctrinated memory can shape a person's actions or even result in delusional disorder.

Mainstream psychiatric and psychological professional associations now harbor strong skepticism towards the notion of recovered memories of trauma. They[kim?] argue that self-help books, such as The Courage to Heal, and recovered memory therapists can influence adults to develop false memories[kaynak belirtilmeli]. According to this theory, psychologists and psychiatrists may accidentally implant these false memories.[kaynak belirtilmeli] The American Psychiatric Association and American Medical Association condemn such practices, whether they are formally called "Recovered Memory Therapy" or simply a collection of techniques that fit the description. In 1998, the Royal College of Psychiatrists Working Group on Reported Recovered Memories of Sexual Abuse wrote:[16]

No evidence exists for the repression and recovery of verified, severely traumatic events, and their role in symptom formation has yet to be proved. There is also striking absence in the literature of well-corroborated cases of such repressed memories recovered through psychotherapy. Given the prevalence of childhood sexual abuse, even if only a small proportion are repressed and only some of them are subsequently recovered, there should be a significant number of corroborated cases. In fact there are none.

That such techniques have been used in the past is undeniable. Their continued use is cause for malpractice litigation worldwide.[17] An Australian psychologist was de-registered for engaging in them.[18]

Evidence for

Human memory is created and highly suggestible, and a wide variety of innocuous, embarrassing and frightening memories can be falsely created through the use of different techniques, including guided imagery, hypnosis and suggestion by others. Though not all individuals who are exposed to these techniques will develop memories, experiments suggest a significant number of people will, and will actively defend the existence of the events, even if told they were false and deliberately implanted. The questions about the possibility of false memories created an explosion of interest in suggestibility of human memory and resulted in an enormous increase in the knowledge about how memories are encoded, stored and recalled, producing pioneering experiments such as the lost in the mall technique.[19] In Roediger and McDermott's (1995) experiment, subjects were presented with a list of related items (such as candy, sugar, honey) to study. When asked to recall the list, participants were just as, if not more, likely to recall semantically related words (such as sweet) than items that were actually studied, thus creating false memories.[20] This experiment, though widely replicated, remains controversial due to debate considering that people may store semantically related items from a word list conceptually rather than as language, which could account for errors in recollection of words without the creation of false memories.

Court cases

Sexual abuse cases

The question of the accuracy and dependability of a repressed memory that someone has later recalled has contributed to some investigations and court cases, including cases of alleged sexual abuse or child sexual abuse(CSA).[21][22] Some such recollections have been supported by enough corroborating evidence to enable successful prosecution,[23] while others have been deemed confabulations or "false memories" that were not legally admissible.[kaynak belirtilmeli]

The defense in sexual abuse cases may offer their own "expert testimony to counter the plaintiff's scientific evidence that the mind can avoid or repress traumatic information and then recall it years later." Murphy believes that there is "overwhelming evidence that the mind is capable of repressing traumatic memories of child sexual abuse."[24] Whitfield states that the "false memory" defense is "seemingly sophisticated, but mostly contrived and often erroneous." He states that this defense has been created by "accused, convicted and self-confessed child molesters and their advocates" to try to "negate their abusive, criminal behavior."[25] Brown states that when pro-false memory expert witnesses and attorneys state there is no causal connection between CSA and adult psychopathology, that CSA doesn't cause specific trauma-related problems like borderline and dissociative identity disorder, that other variables than CSA can explain the variance of adult psychopathology and that the long-term effects of CSA are non-specific and general, that this testimony is inaccurate and has the potential of misleading juries.[26]

Malpractice cases

During the late 1990s, there were multiple lawsuits in the United States in which psychiatrists and psychologists were successfully sued, or settled out of court, on the charge of propagating iatrogenic memories of childhood sexual abuse, incest and satanic ritual abuse.[27]

Some of these suits were brought by individuals who later deemed their recovered memories of incest and/or satanic ritual abuse to be false. The False Memory Syndrome Foundation uses the term "retractors" to describe these individuals and have shared their stories publicly.[28] There is debate regarding the total number of retractions as compared to the total number of allegations,[3] and the reasons for retractions.[29]

See also

Footnotes

  1. ^ McHugh 2008, ss. 66–7.
  2. ^ a b McHugh 2008, s. 55.
  3. ^ a b c Whitfield, Charles L. (2001). Misinformation Concerning Child Sexual Abuse and Adult Survivors. Haworth Press. s. 56. ISBN 0-7890-1901-9.  Bilinmeyen parametre |coauthors= görmezden gelindi (yardım) Kaynak hatası: Geçersiz <ref> etiketi: "Whitfield" adı farklı içerikte birden fazla tanımlanmış (Bkz: Kaynak gösterme)
  4. ^ Appelbaum, Paul S. (1997). Trauma and memory: clinical and legal controversies. Oxford University Press. s. 438. ISBN 0-19-510065-4.  Bilinmeyen parametre |coauthors= görmezden gelindi (yardım)
  5. ^ Rix, Rebecca (2000). Sexual abuse litigation: a practical resource for attorneys, clinicians, and advocates. Routledge. s. 33. ISBN 0-7890-1174-3. 
  6. ^ Gilmore, Leigh (2001). The limits of autobiography: trauma and testimony. Cornell University Press. s. 28. ISBN 0-8014-8674-2. 
  7. ^ Sparta, Steven N. (2006). Forensic mental health assessment of children and adolescents. Oxford University Press. s. 83. ISBN 0-19-514584-4.  Bilinmeyen parametre |coauthors= görmezden gelindi (yardım)
  8. ^ Paterson, H. M., Kemp, R. I., & Forgas, J. P. (2010). "Co-witnesses, confederates, and conformity: The effects of discussion and delay on eyewitness memory.," Psychiatry, Psychology and Law.
  9. ^ Loftus, Elizabeth F. Memory: Surprising New Insights Into How We Remember and Why We Forget (Reading, Mass.: Addison-Wesley Pub. Co., 1980).
  10. ^ Schacter, Daniel L. The Seven Sins of Memory : How the Mind Forgets and Remembers (Houghton Mifflin Co., 2001).
  11. ^ Association for Psychological Science (2008, August 20). "False Memories Affect Behavior."
  12. ^ McHugh 2008, ss. 67–68.
  13. ^ Dallam, S. (2002). "Crisis or Creation: A systematic examination of false memory claims". Journal of Child Sexual Abuse. 9 (3/4): 9–36. doi:10.1300/J070v09n03_02. PMID 17521989. Erişim tarihi: 2008-06-27. 
  14. ^ Dalenberg, C (2006). "Recovered memory and the Daubert criteria: recovered memory as professionally tested, peer reviewed, and accepted in the relevant scientific community". Trauma Violence Abuse. 7 (4): 274–310. doi:10.1177/1524838006294572. PMID 17065548.  Bilinmeyen parametre |author-separator= görmezden gelindi (yardım)
  15. ^ McHugh 2008, s. 63.
  16. ^ Brandon S, Boakes J, Glaser D & Green R (1998). "Recovered memories of childhood sexual abuse: implications for clinical practice". British Journal of Psychiatry. 172: 296–307. 
  17. ^ "Malpractice Suit Against Dr. Bennett Braun". Fortea.us. Erişim tarihi: 2010-12-12. 
  18. ^ "HEALTH CARE COMPLAINTS COMMISSION v TYNAN [2010] NSWPST 1". Austlii.edu.au. 10 February 2010. Erişim tarihi: 2010-12-12. 
  19. ^ Schacter, DL (2002). The Seven Sins of Memory: How the Mind Forgets and Remembers. Houghton Mifflin Harcourt. ss. 123–30. ISBN 0-618-21919-6. 
  20. ^ Roediger, Henry L. (July 1995). "Creating False Memories: Remembering Words Not Presented in Lists". Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition. 4. 21: 803–814.  Bilinmeyen parametre |coauthors= görmezden gelindi (yardım);
  21. ^ "Are Recovered Memories Reliable?". Religioustolerance.org. Erişim tarihi: 2010-12-12. 
  22. ^ Colleen Born. "Elizabeth Loftus". Muskingum.edu. Erişim tarihi: 2010-12-12. 
  23. ^ "The Recovered Memory Project". Brown.edu. 1993-05-03. Erişim tarihi: 2010-12-12. 
  24. ^ Murphy, W. "Debunking 'false memory'myths in sexual abuse cases". 2008-01-07 tarihinde kaynağından arşivlendi. Erişim tarihi: 2008-01-10. 
  25. ^ Whitfield, C. (March 2002). "The "False Memory" Defense Using Disinformation and Junk Science In and Out of Court". Journal of Child Sexual Abuse. 9 (3/4): 53–78. doi:10.1300/J070v09n03_04. Erişim tarihi: 2008-01-11. 
  26. ^ Brown, D. (2001). "(Mis)representation of the Long-Term Effects of Childhood Sexual Abuse in the Courts". Journal of Child Sexual Abuse. 9 (3/4): 79–107. doi:10.1300/J070v09n03_05. PMID 17521992. Erişim tarihi: 2008-01-28. 
  27. ^ "Recovered Memory Lawsuit Sparks Litigation". Psychiatrictimes.com. Erişim tarihi: 2010-12-12. 
  28. ^ Macdonald, G (1999). [Women Against Women Women Against Women] |chapter-url= değerini kontrol edin (yardım) |chapter-url= eksik başlık (yardım). Making of an Illness: My Experience with Multiple Personality Disorder. Laurentian University Press. s. 111. ISBN 0-88667-045-4.  Birden fazla |chapterurl= ve |chapter-url= kullanıldı (yardım)
  29. ^ Summit, R. (1983). "The child sexual abuse accommodation syndrome". Child Abuse & Neglect. 7 (2): 177–193. doi:10.1016/0145-2134(83)90070-4. 

References

External links

Şablon:Memory